Paula S. Cochran (2005) described
the use of biofeedback in speech-language pathology: "Biofeedback does
not require a computer. Examples of low-tech biofeedback often provided
by clinicians to help their clients monitor airflow include straws,
nasal mirrors, and pinwheels. Low-tech biofeedback for voicing and
loudness include voice-activated toys and lights. The key characteristic
here is that while clients are speaking, they experience consequences
that help them modify their speech. Whether the feedback is visual,
auditory, or tactile, if it happens during the speech act and changes as
a result of the speech act, it can be considered biofeedback" (pp.
200-201).
As speech biofeedback illustrates, biofeedback is not confined to popular modalities like EEG, heart rate variability (HRV), SEMG, and temperature. Practitioners can provide biofeedback training for any activity that can be monitored.
deCharms and colleagues (2005) expanded the boundaries of biofeedback when they provided real-time functional MRI (fMRI) biofeedback for rostral anterior cingulate activity (represented as a virtual flame that brightened and dimmed) to control experimental pain induced by pulses of heat applied to the leg.
Scott Adams (2006), the author of the Dilbert comic strip, struggled with spasmodic dysphonia, in which he could not speak in person using a normal voice. This disorder involves abnormal laryngeal muscle contraction, its cause is unknown, and spontaneous remission is rare. Conventional treatment, which includes Botox injections, speech therapy, and surgery attempts to reduce symptom severity, but not cure, this disorder (Bliznikas & Baredes, 2005).
Adams used multiple strategies to regain social speech, including visualization, affirmation, hypnosis, speech therapy exercises, changing pitch, singing words, and speaking in foreign accents (self-regulation). He adjusted his strategy based on its results (feedback) and actively searched for patterns (self-monitoring) to explain why some strategies were more helpful than others. He achieved a breakthrough when he discovered that he could speak in rhyme and repeatedly practiced this strategy for several days until he largely regained normal speech.
Scott Adams's self-treatment for spasmodic dysphonia contains elements of biofeedback like self-regulation exercises, feedback or knowledge of results, self-monitoring, and practice, and illustrates Shellenberger and Green's (1986) mastery model. His success story illustrates the impressive potential of biofeedback to retrain the neuromuscular system.
Biofeedback can be low-tech, high-tech, or no-tech, as in the case of Scott Adams. Biofeedback is defined by the training process, not by the hardware used. Your conceptual model of biofeedback affects the way you think about
biofeedback and how you define your client's role in training.
This unit covers Definitions of biofeedback (I-A), Concepts
of feedback and control in biological systems (I-C), and
Overview of principles of human learning as they apply to biofeedback
(I-D).
Students completing this unit will be able to discuss:
Birk (1973)
“Biofeedback can be defined as the use of monitoring instruments (usually
electrical) to detect and amplify internal physiologic processes within
the body, in order to make this ordinarily unavailable internal
information available to the individual and literally feed it back to him
in some form.”
Hassett (1978)
Biofeedback is a process that "involves making one aware of very subtle
changes in physiological states in the hope of bringing those processes
under conscious control.”
Basmajian (1979)
“Biofeedback may be defined as the technique of using equipment (usually
electronic) to reveal to human beings some of their internal
physiological events, normal and abnormal, in the form of visual and
auditory signals in order to teach them to manipulate these otherwise
involuntary or unfelt events by manipulating and displaying signals.”
Olton and Noonberg (1980)
“Biofeedback may be defined as any technique which increases the ability
of a person to control voluntarily physiological activities by providing
information about those activities…feedback is information and
biofeedback is information about the state of biological processes.”
Schwartz and Fehmi (1982)
“Biofeedback involves the use of sensitive (e.g., electronic or
electromechanical devices) to measure, process, and indicate (i.e.,
feedback) the ongoing activity of various body processes or conditions of
which the person is usually unaware so that the client, client, or
student may have the opportunity to change and to develop beneficial
control over these body processes.”
Shellenberger and Green (1986)
“Biofeedback training is the process of mastering psychophysiological
self-regulation skills, with the aid of information from a biofeedback
instrument, and is similar to skills learning in any activity such as
sports, music, or education. The essential ingredients of biofeedback
training are those of training in any complex skill: clear goals, rewards
for approximating goals, enough time and practice for learning, proper
instructions, a variety of training techniques, and feedback of
information. The essential ‘ingredients’ of the user of the information
from the biofeedback machine are those of any learning of a complex
skill: consciousness, cognitive understanding, language, positive
expectations, motivation, and positive interaction with the coach,
teacher, or therapist.”
Schwartz and Schwartz (1995)
“As a process, applied biofeedback is: a group of therapeutic procedures
that uses electronic or electromechanical instruments to accurately
measure, process, and feed back, to persons and their therapists
information with educational and reinforcing properties, about their
neuromuscular and autonomic activity, both normal and abnormal, in the
form of analogue or binary, auditory and/or visual feedback signals. Best
achieved with a competent biofeedback professional, the objective are to
help persons develop greater awareness of, confidence in, and an
increases in voluntary control over their physiological processes that
are otherwise outside awareness and/or under less voluntary control, by
first controlling the external signal, and then with internal
psychophysiological cognitions, and/or by engaging in and applying
behaviors to prevent symptom onset, stop it, or reduce it soon after
onset.”
Birbaumer and Flor (1999)
"Applied Psychophysiology is a scientific discipline that uses
noninvasive psychophysiological measurements for clinical and other
applied purposes. As the applied section of psychophysiology it shares
with its mother discipline the scope of understanding and modification of
the relationship between behavior and physiological functions by using
noninvasive physiological recordings. Noninvasive physiological
recordings encompass all the classical psychophysiological variables such
as EEG, MEG (magnetoencephalography), EMG, skin conductance, skin
temperature, blood pressure, heart rate, gastro-intestinal motility,
blood flow and vasomotor variables, endocrinological and biochemical
measures in blood, urine, feces and saliva but also modern imaging
techniques such as optical recording and functional magnetic resonance
imaging (fMRI) and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS).
Psychophysiology and applied psychophysiology never use these methods in
isolation but always simultaneously with psychological/behavioral
measurement techniques.”
"Applied psychophysiology reflects an evolving scientific discipline and
specialty involving understanding and modifying the relationship between
behavior and physiological functions by a variety of methods including
noninvasive physiological measures. The term applied psychophysiology is
a rubric encompassing evaluation, diagnosis, education, treatment, and
performance enhancement."
Schwartz (1999)
“Applied psychophysiology includes a group of interventions and
evaluation methods with the exclusive or primary intentions of
understanding and effecting changes that help humans move toward and
maintain healthier psychophysiological functioning. AP involves helping
people change physiological functioning and psychological function
(measured, theoretical, and potential) and/or to achieve sensorimotor
integration and motor learning within physical rehabilitation. The group
of interventions use all forms of biofeedback, relaxation methods,
breathing methods, cognitive behavioral therapies, client
education, behavioral changes, hypnosis, meditative techniques, and
imagery techniques. In some situations, dietary and other biochemical
(nonmedication) changes and some truth detection research and
applications may be considered under the rubric of applied
psychophysiology. Evaluation methods use all forms of physiological
measurements. The physiological functioning includes but is not limited
to accurately measured changes in skeletal muscles, all autonomic
physiology, breathing measures, biochemistry, electroencephalographic
activity, both normal and abnormal and imaging techniques. Autonomic
measures include electrodermal, skin temperature, blood pressure, heart
rate, gastrointestinal motility, and vasomotor. The interventions need to
be part of or have implications for applications to humans. These could,
but do not need to, involve therapy procedures and/or symptoms of medical
and psychophysiological disorders.”
Common elements of these definitions are that (1) biofeedback is
information about personal, couple, or group psychophysiological performance, (2) this
information is obtained by noninvasive monitoring, (3) this information
is used to help individuals achieve
self-regulation (voluntary self-control), and (4) the learning
process resembles motor skill learning.
We can learn psychophysiological responses through the unconscious
processes of perceptual learning, and classical and operant conditioning,
and the conscious process of relational learning.
Perceptual learning allows us to categorize stimuli and recognize that
we've encountered them before. This unconscious learning process enables a
client to
recognize that her headache is a migraine as opposed to a tension-type
headache.
Classical conditioning is an
unconscious associative learning process that modifies
reflexive (elicited) behavior and prepares us to rapidly respond to future
situations. Classical conditioning underlies
conditioned emotional responses, like anxiety when you see a
Highway Patrol vehicle, and the
placebo response, where client expectancies promote
healing.
An unconditioned stimulus (physical
pain) elicits an unconditioned response
(blood pressure increase) without conscious learning.
Classical conditioning associates a neutral stimulus (dental
visit)
with an unconditioned stimulus (physical
pain). Through repeated pairing, the
neutral stimulus becomes a conditioned stimulus that elicits a
conditioned response (blood pressure
increase).
Extinction occurs when failure of an
unconditioned stimulus (physical pain) to follow the conditioned stimulus
(dental visit) weakens or completely eliminates a conditioned response
(blood pressure increase). Spontaneous recovery
occurs when the conditioned response (blood pressure increase) reappears
after a period of time without exposure to the conditioned stimulus
(dental visit). Stimulus generalization
occurs when the conditioned response is elicited by stimuli (blood draw) that resemble the conditioned stimulus (dental visit).
Stimulus discrimination occurs when
the conditioned response (blood pressure increase) is elicited by one
stimulus (dental visit), but not another (blood draw).
Operant conditioning is an
unconscious associative learning process that modifies the form and occurrence of
an operant behavior (emitted behavior)
by manipulating its
consequences.
Operant conditioning occurs with a situational context. The identifying characteristics of a situation are called its
discriminative stimuli and can include
the physical environment and physical, cognitive, and emotional cues. Discriminative stimuli teach us when to perform operant behaviors.
The Quieting Response is a six-second
relaxation exercise developed by Stroebel
that consists of four stages:
In positive reinforcement, behavior (Quieting
Response practice) is followed
by a positive consequence (feeling good) that increases the likelihood
that your client will use the behavior in situations with similar discriminative
stimuli (distress cues).
In negative reinforcement, behavior (Quieting
Response practice) allows your client to escape or avoid an aversive state (distress), thereby increasing
the likelihood that your client will use the behavior in situations with
similar discriminative stimuli (distress cues).
In operant generalization,
your client performs an operant behavior (Quieting Response) with a new
discriminative stimulus (anger instead of anxiety). In
operant discrimination,
your client performs an operant behavior (Quieting Response) when one
discriminative stimulus (anxiety) is present,
but not during another (calm).
In aversive punishment
(positive punishment), an operant
behavior (Quieting Response) is weakened when followed by an aversive
stimulus (frustration). In response cost
(negative punishment), an operant
behavior (Quieting Response) is weakened when followed by the removal of
a rewarding stimulus (client loses attention for symptoms). In
operant extinction, the frequency of behavior (Quieting
Response) declines when it is not reinforced (client is
unable to achieve calm).
Shaping is an operant procedure where
a clinician rewards (provides praise and auditory and visual feedback)
successive approximations of a final behavior (low surface EMG levels in the
upper trapezius muscles when performing the Quieting Response).
Therapists can teach clients to discriminate their psychophysiological
state (hand temperature) by asking them to estimate its value and then by
confirming or correcting their estimate.
Peper has recommended this strategy to teach
clients to estimate their
inhalation volumes (the amount of air they can inhale using an incentive
inspirometer). Clients inhale a volume of air, estimate the volume, and then look at
the actual measurement. This increases client awareness of the depth of
their inhalation.
Relational learning is conscious, allows us to associate stimuli that
occur at the same time, and underlies both working memory (blackboard memory) and long-term declarative memory (memory of facts and experiences). In a
clinical context, it provides conscious memories a client can retrieve
and use to guide behavior (how she calmed herself during meditation). A declarative memory of meditation might simultaneously associate a person's breathing pattern, repeated phrase, and somatosensory feedback (like reduced muscle tone and the slowing of the heart).
Five models have greatly influenced therapist and researcher conceptions
of biofeedback. These include the cybernetic, operant conditioning, drug,
placebo, and relaxation models.
The cybernetic model proposes that "biofeedback is like a thermostat."
The term, biofeedback, originated in cybernetic theory. The components of
a thermostat system include a setpoint or goal (75 degrees),
system
variable or what is controlled (room temperature),
negative feedback or
corrective instructions (commands to change furnace output), and
positive
feedback or commands to continue action (commands to continue furnace
output).
The internal environment fluctuates around a setpoint and is never
stationary. This allows rapid adaptation to changing activity levels and
environmental conditions. Homeostasis is
a state of dynamic constancy achieved by stabilizing
conditions above and below a setpoint, which may change over time. From
the perspective of the cybernetic model, biofeedback training supplements
a client's proprioception to bring a malfunctioning biological system
variable (blood pressure) under better control (Fox, 2006).
Homeostasis depends on sensory systems (networks that monitor system variables) to detect actual or anticipated
change in physiological processes (temperature), an
integrating center,
which receives input from many sensors, and multiple effector systems (control systems) to
adjust physiological processes.
The body maintains dynamic constancy through continuous negative feedback
loops amplified by positive feedback and antagonistic effectors.
Negative feedback loops produce corrective changes when a physiological
variable is outside an acceptable range. For example, we initiate
clotting to stop blood loss from a wound.
Positive feedback amplifies the changes produced by negative feedback.
For example, activation of one clotting factor activates others to create
a blood clot.
Push-pull control by effectors that produce antagonistic effects achieves
more precise control than turning a single effector on or off. For
example, a sympathetic nerve accelerates the heart while the
parasympathetic vagus nerve slows the heart.
The operant conditioning model proposes that "biofeedback is the operant
conditioning of physiological processes." From this perspective,
voluntary changes (reduced muscle bracing) are strengthened by
reinforcing consequences (feedback display). This model implies that
awareness of which change is being reinforced is unnecessary and
disregards the instructional elements that are critical to training
success.
The operant model has several problems. First, operant conditioning is
only one of several learning processes involved in self-regulation
training. Classical conditioning, cognitive learning, motor learning, and
social learning may also be involved.
Second, reinforcement without systematic instruction is an inefficient
way to teach a skill. This would be like a track coach who announced a
sprinter's time, but never demonstrated technique or corrected her form.
The drug model asserts that "biofeedback treatment corrects symptoms like
a drug." This model is implied when a client receives only three
sessions of temperature biofeedback regardless of whether she can warm
her hands to 95 degrees F on command.
The drug model is counterproductive because it places the client in a
passive role (analogous to taking aspirin) and emphasizes dosage (the
number of sessions) over skill mastery. If you believe that skill mastery
affects treatment outcome, then clients should be trained to criterion
and not be limited to an arbitrary number of sessions.
Therapists using the placebo model believe that "biofeedback produces
nonspecific effects, like a drug, due to client beliefs."
While the "laying on of electrodes" undoubtedly produces symptom
improvement, this model discounts the contribution of skill learning.
Second, the changes produced through biofeedback, like increased
end-tidal CO2, can be very specific and are due to modified breathing
mechanics instead of client beliefs.
The relaxation model views biofeedback as inherently relaxing. Based on
this model, therapists may administer biofeedback without relaxation
instructions and researchers may compare biofeedback to relaxation
procedures like Progressive Relaxation.
This approach suffers from serious misconceptions about biofeedback.
First, feedback about your physiology is not always relaxing. For
example, a client told that her blood pressure is elevated will not be
calmed by this news.
Second, whether biofeedback training produces cultivated low arousal
depends on relaxation instructions and the client's approach to
learning. For example, an extreme Type A client learning to lower SEMG
levels could exacerbate stress symptoms by competing against the
electromyograph.
Finally, even traditional relaxation procedures like Autogenic exercises
can produce distress. Relaxation-induced anxiety has been reported for up
to 40% of clients receiving relaxation training.
Blanchard and Epstein (1978) and Shellenberger and Green (1986) developed
models of self-regulation that emphasize the mastery of self-regulation
skills. These approaches are consistent with the view that biofeedback is
information. From this perspective, a therapist coaches the
client to
use information about physiological performance to achieve voluntary
control to reduce symptoms and promote optimal functioning.
Blanchard and Epstein (1978) proposed that self-regulation consists of
five components. Self-monitoring is scanning yourself in a situation
(checking your breathing during a job interview).
Discrimination means
identifying when self-regulation skills should be used based on
situational (stressful confrontation) and internal (rapid heart rate)
cues.
Self-control is the use of a skill to achieve a desired state (practicing
effortless breathing to lower arousal). Self-reinforcement is use of
internal (self-praise) or external rewards (clothing) for use of a skill.
Finally, self-maintenance is long-term skill practice which is aided by
regular review.
In the context of this self-regulation model, successful hypertension
clients:
After six months practice, cues like red traffic lights and cold hands
can automatically trigger self-regulation responses. They become habits
or attractors. Now, a healthy lifestyle becomes rewarding and cheating
becomes aversive.
Shellenberger and Green (1986) advanced a mastery model that compares
biofeedback training to coaching an athletic skill. This training process
is social, since you are working with another person or group, and
biobehavioral, since this instruction uses behavioral principles to
control biological processes.
The biofeedback training process contains these components:
.
Mere exposure to information about our physiological performance produces
no dependable effects. For example, viewing a stopwatch does not reliably
improve a runner's form.
The effect of information depends on how it is used. When biofeedback is
combined with physical therapy, it becomes biofeedback-assisted
rehabilitation. When combined with relaxation training, it becomes
biofeedback-assisted relaxation. Without rehabilitation or relaxation
coaching, the information is neither rehabilitating or relaxing.
Don't assume that biofeedback requires hardware. Information about your
personal biological activity can be detected with or without hardware.
Natural proprioception (bodily sensory feedback) will be your
client's
main source of information after learning to scan the body. Examples:
touch your finger to your wrist to detect pulse rate or touch your cheek
to detect hand temperature.
During training, you can supplement proprioception with high technology
or low technology instruments that monitor performance. Below is a
BioGraph ® Infiniti heart rate variability (HRV) display.
Don't confine
your concept of biofeedback to the traditional modalities like EEG, SEMG,
skin conductance, and temperature. While invaluable in many clinical
applications, your client may not require biomedical devices.
Peper and Shambaugh (1979) have suggested that ordinary devices can provide valuable
performance feedback. For example, clients can correct their facial
expression during public speaking by practicing with a mirror. They can
improve their balance by training with a pair of bathroom scales. They
can reduce hand tremor by using a graduated series of bells. Finally,
athletes can refine their tennis serve using a camcorder for video
feedback.
The training process is social whether a client works alone with a
therapist or within a group. A client's relationship with the therapist
may be the most critical aspect of training.
Taub and School (1978) reported a person effect when teaching
hand-warming. An "informal and friendly" trainer successfully taught 19 of 21 subjects to raise their
finger temperature. In contrast, a more formal and "impersonal" trainer only succeeded
with 2 of 22 subjects (p. 617). The interpersonal dynamics that make psychotherapy
successful are equally important in biofeedback training.
Unlike stereotypical "out-of-shape" coaches with "beer guts," biofeedback
therapists have to develop and practice the skills they are teaching.
Peper (1994) strongly argued that therapists must be "selfexperienced."
Would you be confident with a therapist who looked "stressed out" and
extended an ice-cold handshake?
Therapists learn to self-regulate so they can model self-regulatory
behaviors (like low arousal), personally know that training works, and
understand their clients' learning experiences. Since modeling involves
implicit (unconscious) learning, a therapist who sits tensely or breathes
rapidly may inadvertently teach these behaviors to a client.
An effective biofeedback therapist is a good coach. As in athletic
coaching, successful biofeedback training requires clear goals, graduated
rewards for success, sufficient time and practice for mastery, effective
instructions delivered using systematic training techniques, useful
feedback about performance, and practice of skills outside of the clinic.
Client motivation is often complex and may involve the same
approach-avoidance and avoidance-avoidance conflicts seen in
psychotherapy.
A client has an approach-avoidance conflict when aversive symptoms are
also advantageous (gaining professional and family attention, control
over relationships, and financial compensation). The rewards of
symptomatic behavior are called secondary gains.
An avoidance-avoidance conflict exists when symptoms are unpleasant and
biofeedback training is negatively perceived due to financial cost, time
investment, or social stigma.
Finally, as in psychotherapy, significant others may sabotage training to
maintain their existing relationship. This is particularly a problem in
co-dependent relationships.
You can assess and modify motivation by focusing on the symptoms that
concern your clients. R. Adam Crane asks his
clients during assessment
to select three symptoms they want to improve. The clients are then
asked to chart these symptoms daily throughout the course of biofeedback
training. His criteria for success are a 25% to 35% decrease in symptom
frequency and severity.
This protocol increases client motivation three ways. First, involving
clients in treatment decisions increases their commitment. Second,
involving clients in treatment decisions and assigning responsibilities
places them in an active role and increases perceived self-efficacy.
Third, daily charting of symptoms should produce improvement on its own,
independent of skill mastery.
Successful clients are active participants in this leaning process. A
therapist has an opportunity to modify client expectations about their
role in biofeedback training through educational literature, the
assessment process, and initial training sessions.
Models that emphasize active skill learning, like Shellenberger and
Green's mastery model, may produce better clinical outcomes than those
that promote passivity.
Therapists routinely assign clients skill practice outside of their
training sessions. These assignments often fall into five areas:
lifestyle modification, biofeedback practice, abbreviated relaxation
exercises, deep relaxation exercises, and self-monitoring.
Lifestyle modification involves changes in
routine behavior to help achieve treatment goals. For example, a client treated for anxiety may reduce
caffeine intake from 4 cups of caffeinated coffee to 1 cup of
decaffeinated tea.
Biofeedback practice means continuing biofeedback training with portable
monitors. This is designed to increase a client's proficiency in
self-regulation and generalize it to settings outside of the clinic. For
example, a migraine client may practice hand-warming at home using a
portable feedback thermometer or disposable thermometer.
Abbreviated relaxation exercises like Stroebel's Quieting Response (QR) often
require less than a minute and may be repeated many times a day to promote their acquisition and generalization. They are designed to replace symptoms like anxiety with more adaptive behaviors like cultivated low arousal or mindfulness. Unlike deep relaxation exercises, they can be
easily performed when performing routine activities like commuting..
Deep relaxation exercises like Autogenic Training, meditation, and
Progressive Relaxation require 15 minutes to several hours and involve a
break from routine activity. For example, you could not safely meditate
as you drove your car on an interstate highway. These exercises may
profoundly reduce physiological arousal and reset physiological activity
to healthier values. As a client can more readily experience lowered
arousal, this may accelerate biofeedback training and increase the
effectiveness of abbreviated relaxation exercises.
Self-monitoring means checking symptoms or performance outside of the
clinic. Therapists often ask clients to chart this information to
identify causal patterns, teach them when to use an abbreviated
relaxation exercise to disrupt an escalating symptom, and measure change
in performance.
Research indicates that clients succeed if they occasionally practice
self-regulation skills. Why is practice crucial to client success?
Practice helps a client acquire skills through more time on task. If a
client trains 1 hour a week in the clinic, this leaves 167 hours
available outside the clinic to refine skills learned during the training
session.
Practice helps clients transfer self-regulation skills to environmental
settings. Hand-warming in the clinic does not automatically generalize to
driving in rush hour traffic. Unless clients practice these skills in
the settings where they need them, they might only be symptom-free in the
clinic.
Finally, practice makes self-regulation automatic. For example, when you
learn to drive a manual transmission, you start out concentrating
intensely on shifting, afraid that you will "strip the gears." After
months of practice and several gear boxes, you shift without attention.
Likewise, clients practicing Stroebel's Quieting Response perform this
skill automatically after about 6 months.
Successful biofeedback training teaches voluntary control. Voluntary
control is shown when a person can produce a requested physical change
(warm your hand to 95 degrees F) on command without external feedback.
Voluntary control is achieved through passive and active volition. Passive volition means allowing your body to perform instead of trying to
force it. Forcing defeats your purpose when urinating during intermission
in a movie theater, having an erection, or performing Zen archery.
Active volition means forcing your body. This process is triggered by words like make or try. This is used in conjunction with
passive volition in modern versions of Jacobson's Progressive Relaxation where clients are instructed to intentionally contract and relax muscle
groups to detect and reduce residual muscle tension.
Despite identification of biofeedback training with passive volition,
voluntary control actually involves flexibly shifting between these modes
as required.
Schultz, who developed Autogenic Training, believed that imagery is the
language the body best understands. He contended that an image serves as
a blueprint for physiological change.
Research on successful self-regulators has shown that they use diverse
strategies, including pictures, sounds, bodily sensations, feelings, and
abstract concepts. You should encourage your clients to experiment with
different strategies and then use the ones that work. This communicates
respect for them as collaborators and increases their perceived self-efficacy, which is their perceived ability to achieve desired outcomes.
Both your conceptualization of a disorder and treatment design should be
guided by the clinical literature. This can be frustrating for a
clinician when research findings contradict conventional wisdom about a
disorder. Evidence-Based Practice in Biofeedback and Neurofeedback, which is published by the Assocation for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, summarizes clinical efficacy studies for diverse biofeedback/neurofeedback applications and should often be consulted first when designing treatment plans.
The discredited sympathetic arousal model of Raynaud's disease
provides an excellent example. The sympathetic arousal model asserts that stressors are powerful
triggers for Raynaud's attacks and that interventions that lower
sympathetic arousal will reduce symptom severity. Therapists who
subscribe to this model usually provide biofeedback to lower sympathetic
tone and teach stress management skills.
Research by Freedman and colleagues has challenged the sympathetic
arousal model's assumptions. Raynaud's disease appears to be due to a
"local fault" in peripheral blood vessels. Cold and cold-related stimuli
are more likely to trigger hand-cooling than stressors. Temperature
biofeedback does not prevent attacks by reducing sympathetic arousal.
Finally, bidirectional temperature biofeedback with cold challenge produces greater symptom reduction than stress management techniques. In this strategy, clinicians teach clients to alternatively warm or cool their extremities within a cold room or while wearing a suit cooled by circulating cold water.
Failure to revise our models and treatment strategies may seriously
reduce our clinical effectiveness.
Clients reporting a symptom, like hypertension, may have very different
psychophysiological profiles. Treatment should be personalized to correct
abnormalities: values that are too high, low, show excessive or
insufficient variability, or recover too slowly.
Biofeedback training of astronauts and pilots to reduce motion sickness illustrates this approach. A therapist monitors an individual's
autonomic responses during stress testing, in a Barany chair that tilts and rotates or in a centrifuge, and identifies
the systems that respond abnormally. Biofeedback-assisted relaxation to correct abnormal responses can prevent or moderate motion sickness (Cowings et al., 1986; Cowings & Toscano, 1982).
Personalizing treatment also means training clients to mastery criteria.
For example, you might provide temperature training until your client
can achieve 95 degrees F without feedback. This is more flexible than
limiting training to five sessions of temperature biofeedback whether the
client has succeeded or not. Clients have different learning curves and
learning styles.
The length and number of sessions should reflect research data concerning
biofeedback training for a specific modality (temperature) and specific
disorder (migraine). Realistically, treatment protocols will be affected
by each client’s learning curve, availability, and ability to pay for
training.
Bidirectional training teaches client to increase and decrease a physiological response. This strategy may be more effective than training in a single
direction. This advantage may result from increased training time, higher
proficiency standards, and learning to control more than one
physiological mechanism.
Bidirectional training has been advocated in both temperature and EEG
biofeedback. When teaching clients to hand-warm to prevent Raynaud's
episodes, you might teach hand-warming and cooling in the same or
successive sessions. In neurofeedback training to control pain by
increasing theta activity, ON-OFF-ON protocols (theta increase-theta
suppression-theta increase) may produce the best results.
Wherever feasible outside of neuromuscular rehabilitation, biofeedback training should be spaced instead of
massed. In spaced practice, training sessions are scheduled over an
extended period of time to produce greater skill acquisition and
generalization. A 15-session training protocol might involve 2 weekly
sessions for 5 weeks and then 1 session a week for the remaining 5 weeks.
In massed practice, training sessions are compressed into a shorter time
period. A client might receive 5 sessions a week for 3 weeks. This
training schedule might be necessary when a client is hospitalized
(constraint-induced movement therapy for stroke) or treated as an out-client in
another city.
Biofeedback therapists use multiple treatment strategies to aid
generalization of self-regulation skills to life situations:
Olton and Noonberg (1980) advised that performance goals should be raised
when a client succeeds more than 70% of the time and lowered when a
client succeeds less than 30% of the time. Computer-based biofeedback
systems incorporate algorithms like the 70-30 rule and automatically
revise goals based on the client's performance during the previous 15-
or 30-second trial.
This approach helps maintain client motivation and insure sufficient
challenge. Training success can increase the client's perception of
self-efficacy, which may positively affect a
client's symptoms.
How do you explain biofeedback to your clients? Biofeedback metaphors
help define your client's role in the training process. I recommend two
metaphors that portray biofeedback as a process that teaches the client
skills using information about personal performance.
"Biofeedback is like coaching a runner using a stopwatch." This metaphor
emphasizes the importance of coaching in improving client performance.
"Biofeedback is like teaching carpentry, not hammering." This metaphor
communicates that the purpose of biofeedback training is to teach
self-regulation instead of mastery of a particular technique.
70-30 rule: training thresholds should be adjusted
when a client succeeds more than 70% of the time and lowered when a
client succeeds less than 30% of the time.
abbreviated relaxation exercises: procedures like Stroebel's Quieting Response (QR) that produce weak-to-moderate subjective and physiological change, involve minimal sensory restriction, and are practiced for very brief periods of time, that are designed to replace symptoms like anxiety with more adaptive behaviors like cultivated low arousal or mindfulness.
active volition: a process where you direct yourself to perform an action like clenching a fist that is triggered by words like make or try.
approach-avoidance conflict: the client perceives her symptoms as both aversive (discomfort and disability) and advantageous (gaining professional and family attention, control
over relationships, and financial compensation).
Autogenic Training: deep relaxation procedure developed by Schultz and Luthe that involves a sequence of three procedures: six standard
exercises, autogenic modification, and autogenic meditation.
aversive punishment (positive punishment): the weakening of an operant behavior when it is followed by an an aversive stimulus (a client reduces relaxation practice when lack of progress frustrates her).
avoidance-avoidance conflict: the client perceives both her symptoms (low back pain) and training (SEMG biofeedback) as aversive.
bidirectional training: teaching a client to alternatively increase and decrease a physiological response (increase and decrease masseter SEMG).
bidirectional temperature biofeedback with cold challenge: biofeedback training to alternatively increase and decrease peripheral temperature in a cold room or while wearing a cold suit.
biofeedback: information about personal, couple, or group psychophysiological performance that is obtained by noninvasive monitoring and used to help individuals achieve self-regulation through a learning
process that resembles motor skill learning.
classical conditioning: unconscious associative learning process that modifies reflexive behavior and prepares us to rapidly respond to future
situations.
conditioned response (CR): in classical conditioning, a response (blood pressure rise) elicited by a conditioned stimulus (criticism).
conditioned stimulus (CS): in classical conditioning, a stimulus (dentist's office), that in association with an unconditioned stimulus (pain), elicits an unconditioned response (anxiety) like the original unconditioned response.
cultivated low arousal: reduced activation of the central nervous system (EEG) and peripheral nervous system (autonomic and somatic) observed during deep relaxation.
cybernetic model: explanation that biofeedback is like a thermostat system with a setpoint, system variable, and negative and positive feedback.
deep relaxation exercises: procedures like Autogenic Training, meditation, and
Progressive Relaxation that may require 15 minutes to several hours, involve a
break from routine activity, and profoundly reduce physiological arousal and reset physiological activity
to healthier values.
discrimination: perception of psychophysiological performance, like the difference between 0.5 microvolt and 1 microvolt of SEMG activity in the masseter muscles, which is a crucial component of self-regulation.
discriminative stimuli: in operant conditioning, the identifying characteristics of a situation (the physical environment and physical, cognitive, and emotional cues) that teach us when to perform operant behaviors; for example, a traffic slowdown signals a client to practice effortless breathing.
drug model: explanation that biofeedback can be administered like a doses of a drug, which ignores the role of skill mastery and the need to train clients to criteria.
extinction (classical conditioning): the weakening and disappearance of a conditioned response (CR) when it is repeated presented without the unconditioned stimulus (UCS); for example, a client's blood pressure rise (CR) decreases and then disappears after several painless visits (UCS).
extinction (operant conditioning): the weakening and disappearance of an operant behavior when it is no longer reinforced; for example, a client reduces and then stops practicing the Quieting Response after she ceases to feel calm during this exercise.
homeostasis: state of dynamic constancy achieved by stabilizing
conditions above and below a setpoint, which may change over time.
integrating center: in cybernetic theory, the site that receives sensory input; for example, the hypothalamus is the primary integrating center in the human body.
interoception: perception of the body's interior (pain and pressure).
lifestyle modification: changes in
routine behavior like diet and exercise to help achieve treatment goals.
long-term declarative memory: system that creates conscious long-term memories about facts and our experiences (episodic memories).
massed practice: compression of training sessions into a brief period of time like 10 sessions in 2 weeks.
mastery model: Shellenberger and Green's (1986) explanation that compares
biofeedback training to coaching an athletic skill.
multiple effector systems: in cybernetic theory, multiple control systems; for example, the hypothalamus regulates homeostasis through its control over the autonomic nervous system, immune system, and somatic nervous system.
negative feedback: corrective instructions when a physiological
variable is outside an acceptable range (feedback tone).
negative reinforcement: in operant conditioning, the strengthening of an operant behavior (effortless breathing) when it is followed by the avoidance or removal of an aversive stimulus (pain).
neutral stimulus (NS): in classical conditioning, a stimulus (sight of a street light) that does not trigger a conditioned response (CR).
ON-OFF-ON: training paradigm in which a client performs a target behavior (increases alpha amplitude), suppresses it (reduces alpha amplitude), and then produces it again (increases alpha amplitude),
operant behavior: behavior that operates on the environment and is under voluntary control (writing in a personal journal).
operant conditioning: unconscious associative learning process that modifies the form and occurrence of
voluntary behavior
by manipulating its
consequences.
operant conditioning model: explanation that "biofeedback is the operant
conditioning of physiological processes," which minimizes the importance of client awareness of physiological changes and instruction in training.
operant generalization: an operant behavior is performed when a new discriminative stimulus is present (client breathes effortlessly when both anxious or experiencing pain).
passive volition: a process where you invite yourself to perform an action like dropping your arm in your lap that is triggered by words like allow or permit .
perceptual learning: unconscious learning process that allows us to categorize stimuli and recognize that
we've encountered them before.
person effect: Taub and School's (1978) observation that biofeedback training is a social situation and that a client's relationship with the therapist
may be the most critical aspect of training.
placebo model: explanation that "biofeedback produces
nonspecific effects, like a drug, due to client beliefs," which discounts the contribution of skill learning.
positive feedback (feedforward): in cybernetic theory, commands to continue action that amplifies the changes produced by negative feedback.
positive reinforcement: in operant conditioning, the strengthening of an operant behavior (aerobic exercise) when it is followed by a reward (praise).
Progressive Relaxation: Jacobson's deep relaxation procedure that originially trained patients to relax 2 or 3 muscle groups each session until 50 groups were trained during 50-60 sessions in the clinic and 1-2 daily one-hour practice sessions.
push-pull control: regulation by effectors that produce antagonistic effects and achieves more precise control than turning a single effector on or off; for example, parasympathetic and sympathetic motor neurons jointly adjust the heart rhythm.
Quieting Response (QR): Strobel's (1982) 6-second exercise which instructs a client to focus on a stress cue, smile inwardly, take an easy deep breath, and let the jaw, tongue, and shoulders go limp as she exhales.
relational learning: conscious learning process that associates stimuli that occur at the same time and underlies working memory and declarative memory (learning how to warm your hands using a biofeedback display)..
relaxation-induced anxiety: increased anxiety during relaxation training that may include increased perspiration, shivering, trembling, pounding heart, and rapid breathing
relaxation model: explanation that biofeedback is inherently relaxing, which could lead therapists to administer biofeedback without relaxation instructions.
response cost (negative punishment): the weakening of an operant behavior (pain complaints) when it is followed by the removal of a rewarding stimulus (narcotic medication).
secondary gains: the rewards of symptomatic behavior, like reduced housecleaning responsibilities following the display of pain behaviors.
self-control: using a skill to achieve a desired state (running on a treadmill to reduce weight).
self-efficacy: perceived ability to achieve desired outcomes (your belief that you can learn to lower your blood pressure).
self-monitoring: observing yourself in a situation (taking your pulse after a run).
self-maintenance: ensuring long-term skill practice (periodically reviewing your success with effortless breathing and fine-tuning this skill).
self-regulation: control of your behavior (voluntary hand-warming).
self-reinforcement: using internal or external rewards to increase performance of a behavior (praising yourself for using effortless breathing during an argument).
sensory systems: networks that detect actual or anticipated changes in system variables.
setpoint: goal like core body temperature of 98.6 degrees F.
social learning (observational learning): learning process in which observation of the consequences of a model's behavior can influence an individual's operant behavior (exposure to a therapist's slow breathing increases a client's practice of effortless breathing).
spaced practice: scheduling training sessions over an extended period of time like 15 sessions over 8 weeks.
spasmodic dysphonia: the inability to speak in person using a normal voice due to abnormal laryngeal muscle contraction.
spontaneous recovery: in classical conditioning, the reappearance of an extinguished conditioned response (CR) following a rest period; for example, your blood pressure increase returns after 6 months away from the dentist's office).
stimulus discrimination: in classical conditioning, when a conditioned response (CR) is elicited by one conditioned stimulus (CS), but not by another; for example, your blood pressure increases during a painful dental procedure, but not during an uncomfortable blood draw.
stimulus generalization: in classical conditioning, when stimuli that resemble a conditioned stimulus (CS) elicit a conditioned response (CR); for example, when your blood pressure increases during a painful dental procedures and an uncomfortable blood draw.
sympathetic arousal model of Raynaud’s: poorly-supported model that asserts that stressors are powerful
triggers for Raynaud's attacks and that interventions that lower
sympathetic arousal will reduce symptom severity.
system variable: in cybernetic theory, the variable that is controlled, like room temperature.
unconditioned response (UCR): an innate response that is elicited by an unconditioned stimulus (UCS) without prior learning (elevated blood pressure in response to physical pain).
unconditioned stimulus (UCS): a stimulus (physical pain) that elicits an innate response (increased blood pressure) without prior learning.
working memory: short-term conscious memory system that is called "blackboard memory."
Now that you have completed this module, write down your favorite
biofeedback metaphor. Based on your own clinical experience, what would
you add to the discussion of the training process?
Adams, S. (2006, October 24). Good news day. Retrieved November 22, 2006 from http://dilbertblog.typepad.com/the_dilbert_blog/.
Birbaumer, N., & Flor, H. (1999). Applied psychophysiology and learned
physiological regulation. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback,
24(1), 35-37.
Bliznikas, D, & Baredes, S. (2005, August 4). Spasmodic dysphonia. Retrieved November 22, 2006 from http://www.emedicine.com/ENT/topic349.htm.
Carlson, J. G., Seifert, A. R., & Birbaumer, N. (Eds.). (1994). Clinical
applied psychophysiology. New York: Plenum Press.
Cochran, P. S. (2005). Clinical computing competency for
speech-language pathology. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.
Cowings, P. S., & Malmstrom, F. V. (1984). What you thought you knew about motion sickness isn't necessarily so. Flying Safety, 2, 12-17.
Cowings, P. S., Suter, S., Toscano, W. B., Kamiya, J., & Naifeh, K. (1986). General autonomic components of motion sickness. Psychophysiology, 23, 542-551.
Cowings, P. S., & Toscano, W. B. (1982). The relationship of motion sickness susceptibility to learned autonomic control for symptom suppression. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 53, 570-575.
deCharms, R. C., Fumiko, M., Glover, G. H., Ludlow, D., Pauly, J. M., Soneji, D., Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Mackey, S. C. (2005). Control over brain activation and pain learned by using real-time functional MRI. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(51), 18626-18631.
Fox, S. I. (2006) Human physiology (9th ed.). New York: McGraw Hill.
Myers, A., & Hansen, C. (2006). Experimental psychology. Pacific Grove,
CA: Wadsworth.
Olton, D. S., & Noonberg, A. R. (1980). Biofeedback: Clinical
applications in behavioral medicine. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall,
Inc.
Peper, E., & Shambaugh, S. (1979). Economical biofeedback devices.
Mind/body integration: Essential readings in biofeedback. In E.
Peper, S. Ancoli, & M. Quinn (Eds.). New York: Plenum Press, 557-562.
Schwartz, M. S. (1999). Responses to comments and closer to a definition
of applied psychophysiology. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback,
24(1), 43-54.
M. S.
Schwartz, & F. Andrasik (Eds.). (2003). Biofeedback: A practitioner's guide
(3rd ed.).
New
York: The Guilford Press.
Shellenberger, R., & Green, J. A. (1986). From the ghost in the box to
successful biofeedback training. Greeley: Health Psychology Publications.
Taub, E., & School, P. J. (1978). Some methodological considerations in thermal biofeedback training. Behavior Research Methods & Instrumentation, 10(5), 617-622.
Wickramasekera, I. A. (1988). Clinical behavioral medicine: Some concepts
and procedures. New York: Plenum Press.
Wickramasekera, I. A. (1999). How does biofeedback reduce clinical
symptoms and do memories and beliefs have biological consequences? Toward
a model of mind-body healing. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback,
24(2), 91-105.