This unit discusses Chronic neuromuscular pain (IV-C) and
Specific CNS structures (VI-A).
Students completing this unit will be
able to discuss:
Free nerve endings are the body’s
primary nociceptors (pain receptors). There are three main kinds of
nociceptors: mechanoreceptors (mechanical stress and tissue damage),
chemoreceptors (chemical messengers), and thermoreceptors (temperature
extremes). Nociceptors may be unimodal
(respond to only one stimulus) or polymodal
(respond to several stimuli, like C-fibers).
Sherman (2004) estimates that almost 50% of nociceptors located in joints and, possibly, the skin are "silent." These unmyelinated C-fibers only react to stimuli like limb movement and touch when there is local inflammation. Their activation increases pain two-fold.
Painful stimuli often damage tissue, which causes the release of
substance P, bradykinin, histamine, and prostaglandin. Nociceptors
release the peptide substance P through their many axonal branches in nearby skin. Substance P produces
capillary swelling and histamine release from mast cells, sensitizing
other nociceptors located at the injury site. Bradykinin, histamine, and prostaglandin bind to nociceptors and increase their firing rate,
increasing pain. Histamine and the prostaglandins also produce swelling
(Wilson, 2003).
Tissue injury can increase sensitivity to normal stimulation like light touch. The nervous system can increase pain by lowering the pain threshold, increasing pain severity, or generating spontaneous pain. This supersensitivity is called primary hyperalgesia, when it affects joints, muscles, or skin that have suffered damage or inflammation, and secondary hyperalgesia, when it affects surrounding tissue (Bear, Connors, & Paradiso, 2007).
Four main types of afferent axons conduct information crucial to pain
perception: A-alpha, A-beta, A-delta, and C-fibers.
A-alpha (Aα) fibers are
large-diameter myelinated fibers that rapidly conduct (80-120 m/s) muscle
length, contraction force, and light touch stimuli via the
dorsal-column medial-lemniscal and
anterolateral systems.
A-beta (Aβ) fibers are large-diameter myelinated fibers that rapidly conduct
(35-75 m/s) light touch stimuli via the
dorsal-column medial-lemniscal and
anterolateral systems.
A-delta (Aδ) fibers are small-diameter myelinated fibers found in
skin and mucous membranes that strongly influence the
sensory-discriminative component of pain. They rapidly conduct (5-30 m/s) pain signals from unimodal mechanoreceptors via the
anterolateral system to the thalamus and somatosensory cortex shown below. Aδ
fibers carry the first pain signal
that warns that damage has occurred. This sensation is described as
sharp, brief pain.
C-fibers are small-diameter
unmyelinated fibers located in deep tissue and skin that strongly
influence the affective and motivational components of pain. These fibers slowly
conduct (1 m/s) pain signals from polymodal chemoreceptors and thermoreceptors
to the hypothalamus, thalamus, and cortex. C-fibers carry the second pain
signal that reminds the body to protect itself against further
injury. This sensation is described as dull, aching pain.
Afferent fibers from the nociceptors in the skin and viscera (large internal organs) converge and interact in the spinal cord. Cross-talk between these axons produces referred pain like angina, in which visceral pain due to insufficient oxygen to the heart muscle is mainly experienced in the upper chest, but sometimes the back, arms, lower jaw, neck, or shoulders (Bear, Connors, & Paradiso, 2007; Heuther & McCance, 2004).
The combined firing of A-δ and C-fibers results in
double pain, where a patient
experiences two peaks, sharp, brief pain and dull, aching pain (Taylor,
2006).
Axons that carry nociceptive information release glutamate during mild
pain and release both glutamate and substance P during more intense pain
(Kalat, 2004).
There is a complex dynamic interaction between
ascending pathways that distribute pain information to the
brain and descending pathways that
modulate (facilitate or inhibit) incoming pain signals. In theory, disruption of the balance
between the ascending and descending pathways can result in functional
and structural changes in the central nervous system that produce chronic
pain.
The dorsal-column medial-lemniscus system and anterolateral system are
the two major ascending somatosensory pathways. Both systems transmit
information from peripheral receptors to the brain over a three-neuron
pathway that includes a primary afferent axon, a secondary projection
neuron, and a tertiary neuron whose cell body lies in the thalamus.
Interneurons in these ascending pathways receive and modulate messages from nociceptors. A single interneuron may communicate with thousands of other interneurons via excitatory and inhibitory synapses. Chronic pain can modify these complex interneuron networks through repeated stimulation that sensitizes them to inoccuous stimuli. This is particularly a problem when localized inflammation does not remit. In wind-up, a patient may continue to experience pain after an injury has healed because interneurons continue to activate each other in the absence of peripheral pain signals (Sherman, 2004).
The dorsal-column medial-lemniscus system
distributes touch (fine tactile and vibratory) and proprioceptive (joint
and limb position) information to the somatosensory
cortex. Most sensory neurons that contribute to this system have very
small receptive fields that allow precise spatial localization and
project to the spinal cord using large-diameter myelinated Aβ
fibers. Visceral (large internal) organs provide pain information via
C-fibers that enter the spinal cord and synapse on dorsal horn neurons.
Sensory neurons enter the spinal cord through the
dorsal root, ascend in the
dorsal columns on the same side, and
synapse with dorsal column nuclei in the medulla.
Axons from dorsal column nuclei cross
over to the opposite side and ascend via the
medial lemniscus to the ventral
posterior nucleus (VPN) of the thalamus. The VPN also
integrates somatosensory information from the the opposite side of the
face via the trigeminal nerve. The
majority of VPN axons target the primary
somatosensory cortex (SI), while a minority target the secondary
somatosensory cortex (SII) or the posterior parietal cortex.
The anterolateral system (ALS) distributes
pain and temperature information to the somatosensory cortex and other
brain regions. The main afferent axons that comprise the ALS are
small-diameter Aδ and C-fibers. Since the
sensory neurons that contribute to this system have large receptive
fields, spatial localization is imprecise. The majority of these sensory neurons synapse within the spinal
cord and most of their second-order neurons cross over and ascend via the
contralateral anterolateral spinal cord. A minority of these neurons do not
cross over and, instead, ascend on the same side.
The anterolateral system contains the spinothalamic,
spinoreticular, spinomesencephalic, and spinohypothalamic tracts,
and receives information about the face from the three branches of the
trigeminal nerve.
The spinothalamic tract (also called
the neospinothalamic tract) targets
the ventral posterior nucleus (VPN) of the thalamus, like the
dorsal-column medial-lemniscus system. VPN neurons project to
corresponding somatosensory cortical region. This tract may convey
information about pain and temperature.
The spinoreticular tract (also called
the paleospinothalamic tract) targets the
reticular formation and then the thalamic parafascicular nuclei and
intralaminar nuclei. Axons from these thalamic nuclei project to the
anterior cingulate, amygdala, and hypothalamus. This tract may contribute
to arousal and convey
information about our emotional response to pain, including suffering.
The spinomesencephalic tract
travels with the spinotectal tract and targets the periaqueductal gray
(PAG), locus coeruleus, and tectum. This tract may convey information
concerning motivational and emotional responses to pain.
The spinohypothalamic tract bypasses
the reticular formation and projects to the hypothalamus, amygdaloid
complex, nucleus accumbens, and septum. This tract may contribute to our
autonomic, neuroendocrine, and emotional responses to noxious stimuli.
The trigeminal nerve projects pain
and temperature information concerning the face to the same thalamic
nuclei targeted by the spinothalamic and spinoreticular tracts.
The thalamic nuclei targeted by these tracts, in turn, project pain and temperature information to
SI, SII, the posterior parietal cortex, and other brain regions
(Kingsley, 2000; Pinel, 2006).
Several descending systems modulate the transmission of pain information.
Ranney (2001) speculates that there
are five bidirectional loops between the reticular formation and the
spinal cord (spinoreticular and reticulospinal) on each side of the body.
These loops transmit information in both directions and can either
facilitate or inhibit pain transmission. These loops connect the spinal
cord to the dorsolateral pons, medulla, and hypothalamus. Stimulation of
these loops can affect pain transmission from hours to days.
The cerebral cortex modulates pain
transmission via projections to the thalamus and reticular formation. The
locus coeruleus more directly
modulates pain transmission by its projections to the dorsal horn of the
spinal cord.
Periaqueductal gray (PAG) axons form
excitatory synapses on neurons located in the
nucleus raphe magnus (NRM) and the
nucleus reticularis paragigantocellularis (NRPG) in the
medulla. The NRM and NRPG are part of the reticular formation. Their
axons enter the spinal cord and synapse on interneurons in the dorsal
horn.
Most NRM neurons excite spinal cord interneurons by releasing serotonin.
These interneurons, in turn, inhibit C-fibers by releasing enkephalin,
which binds to their terminal buttons and reduces their release of
transmitter, a process called presynaptic inhibition.
NRPG neurons excite spinal cord interneurons by releasing norepinephrine.
These interneurons inhibit neurons that project to the thalamus through a
polysynaptic pathway that does not release opioids.
PAG neurons are influenced by diverse projections, including the
ascending spinomesencephalic tract of the ALS and descending axons from
the cerebral cortex, thalamus, and hypothalamus.
Mu (μ)
receptors on PAG neurons allow both opioids that are released
synaptically, extra-synaptically through volume transmission, and through
drug administration to influence their activity (Kingsley, 2000).
Melzack and Wall (1965) were the first investigators to explain why
people respond in different ways under different circumstances to
identically painful stimuli. Their Gate-Control
Theory of Pain represented an early pattern theory of how we respond to and control our
response to painful stimuli. They conceptualized pain as a type of
cutaneous (skin) sensation that arises from somatosensory signals
converging on dorsal horn neurons (in the spinal cord) that communicate
with an adjustable pattern detector in the thalamus and cortices.
This
was the first model to propose cognitive-emotional mediation of pain and
an interaction between ascending and descending pathways, instead of the
simplistic one-way model where pain signals simply ascend from the spinal cord
to the brain.
In the Gate-Control Theory of Pain,
C-fibers carry information about pain
to neurons in the substantia gelatinosa, which is located in the dorsal
horn of the spinal cord and extends into the medulla. From the substantia
gelatinosa, information about pain is relayed to the brain stem, and then to
the cerebral cortex, where pain is consciously experienced.
Gatchel (2005) believes that the pain
"gate" is opened by pain-centered thoughts and negative thinking;
negative feelings like anger, helplessness, hopelessness, sadness, and
stress; and behaviors like inactivity, insufficient sleep, poor diet, and
smoking.
Axons from the periaqueductal gray (PAG)
and the periventricular gray areas
release pain-inhibiting neurotransmitters in the substantia gelatinosa to
"close the gate.”
Aβ fibers that carry tactile
information and coping behaviors can "close the gate" to stop pain
information from reaching the brain (Gatchel, 2005).
Pain receptors use a neurochemical known as
substance P to signal the presence of tissue damage and pain
to the central nervous system. Substance P is released by small-diameter,
unmyelinated C-fibers in the substantia gelatinosa and excites neurons
whose axons carry information about pain to the brain. However, centers
in the cerebrum and brain stem project axons down to the substantia
gelatinosa. These axons can release endorphins
and nonopiate transmitters like
norepinephrine and serotonin to produce
analgesia (the absence of pain) by
inhibiting C-fiber release of substance P in the substantia gelatinosa
via presynaptic inhibition (Wilson, 2003).
The stress response can produce analgesia that is not reversed by the
opioid-blocker naloxone. ACTH, corticosterone, and neurotensin can reduce
pain without the release of opioids.
Treatments for pain appear to activate the endorphin or the nonopiate
pain-inhibitory systems, or both. These treatments include drugs, counterirritation, acupuncture, transcutaneous electrical nerve
stimulation (TENS), and hypnosis.
Craig (2003) proposed, in contrast
to pattern theorists like Melzack and Wall, that pain is a unique
homeostatic emotion concerned with the preservation of the body. He
challenged the Gate-Control Theory of Pain by observing that neither
somatosensory cortical damage nor stimulation alters pain, that chronic
pain can be reduced by stimulation of the somatosensory thalamus, and
that in primates pain is a feeling produced by separate sensory channels
that directly project via a central homeostatic
afferent pathway from the thalamus to the cortex. These
thalamocortical projections help us create an image of the body's
current physiological state (sensation) and directly activate the limbic
motor cortex (affective motivation). Specifically, a sensation is
generated by the interoceptive and anterior
insular cortex, "the feeling self." Affective motivation is
produced by the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC),
the "behavioral agent." Thus, pain consists of both a specific sensation
and affective motivation to initiate autonomic and motor responses to a
condition that threatens body integrity.
Pain perception has sensory-discriminative, motivational-affective, and
cognitive-evaluative dimensions. The
sensory-discriminative component
of pain perception, which determines what the pain feels like (dull
aching or sharp), is processed in the secondary
somatosensory cortex. The
motivational-affective component,
which determines the pain's unpleasantness, is processed in the anterior cingulate cortex
of the frontal lobe. The cognitive-evaluative
component, which determines what the
pain means (GI distress or angina), is processed in the
prefrontal cortex and the
insula (Wilson, 2003).
Eisenberger, Lieberman, and Williams (2003)
used a functional MRI (fMRI) to study the brains of subjects who believed that two
companions playing an on-line baseball simulation suddenly dropped
them from the game. Their emotional distress activated the anterior
cingulate cortex, which evaluates the unpleasantness of physical pain.
Acute pain is pain associated with
tissue damage, involves brief, intense unpleasant sensations, warns of
potential injury, and may produce transient anxiety. The pain experience
is easily recognized and localized. Healing and treatment are effective
about 90% of the time.
Chronic pain is pain that has persisted for at least 6 months (longer than the 2-4 months usually required for healing) and has not responded to medical treatment. There is a weak relationship between physical findings and pain severity. Chronic pain disrupts patients' daily activities. They may become preoccupied with their pain, increase health care utilization (testing, treatment, and medication), and present with anger, depression, and frustration.
Episodic or
recurrent pain is pain that has recurred for more than 3
months. Like acute pain, these episodes are brief. However, these
episodes often produce psychological distress (like chronic pain) and
patients may present with anxiety, depression, helplessness, and stress.
While pain medication may reduce pain severity, it usually does not
prevent recurrent episodes. Prolonged episodes of low back or headache
pain may resemble chronic pain syndromes in symptoms, psychological
distress, and degree of incapacitation (Gatchel, 2005).
Respondent pain is pain due
to an identifiable stimulus, like a dental drill. This pain does not
require previous learning. Operant pain
is the result of previous reinforcement of pain behaviors by the
environment (secondary gain).
Pain behaviors persist after injury has healed. The patient
assumes a role that is reinforced by a health care system that treats
chronic pain as if it were acute.
Wickram's Illness Behavior Syndrome (1988)
consists of five components:
The topographical map of the primary somatosensory cortex is quite
plastic and can be reorganized. The topographical organization of the
primary somatosensory cortex is altered following amputation of a limb.
The ability of the primary somatosensory cortex to reorganize may
contribute to the experience of chronic pain, especially a type of
chronic pain known as phantom limb pain.
In phantom limb pain, a person who has had a limb amputated continues to
feel pain in the missing limb long after it has been amputated. The
severity of phantom limb pain is directly related to the extent of the
reorganization of the somatosensory cortex, with more pain associated
with greater cortical reorganization (Wilson, 2003).
Melzack's (1992)
neuromatrix model of phantom limb pain proposes that a
widely-distributed neural network responds to sensory stimulation and generates a signal
that the body is intact and one's own. When there are no sensory signals
from the periphery, this neurosignature produces the
false perception that
the patient still has a limb (even after amputation). Almost 70 percent
of amputees report phantom burning, cramping, or shooting pain as part of
their body although the limb is missing.
The neuromatrix consists of three neural systems: the sensory pathway from the thalamus to the somatosensory cortex, emotional and motivational pathways from the brain stem reticular formation to the limbic system, and cortical regions that recognize the self and process sensory input found mainly in the parietal lobe. Phantoms in patients born without limbs suggest that the neuromatrix may be mostly prewired. Experience may modify the connections which resemble Hebb's cell assemblies.
Sensory signals
entering the brain pass through all three systems in parallel, they share
their analysis, and send an integrated evaluation to other brain regions.
(The site where this output becomes completely conscious is unknown.)
This output includes an evaluation of the sensory information and a
neurosignature (message that the signals come from our own body).
The neuromatrix model hypothesizes that peripheral sensory stimulation or
CNS processes can generate specific patterns of neural network firing
that create a multidimensional pain experience. The neuromatrix develops
through an interplay of genetics, sensory experience, and learning. This
is a diathesis-stress model of pain.
A person's vulnerability to pain (diathesis) interacts with acute
stressors to trigger neural network firing patterns that we experience as
pain. Pain, in turn, can become a powerful chronic stressor (Gatchel,
2005).
Injury that threatens homeostasis is stressful and can activate Selye's
hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis,
releasing cortisol. Prolonged
cortisol release can metabolize new muscle protein and inhibit calcium
replacement in bones, resulting in joint and muscle pain (Gatchel, 2005).
This model explains common complaints of burning and cramping in the phantom limb. When the neuromatrix loses sensory input from a limb, this may produce high levels of neuromatrix firing (like thalamic bursting) which may produce perceptions of burning. Failure of a limb to move in response to neuromatrix commands may result in more intense neuromatrix firing which may be perceived as cramping or shooting pain.
This phenomenon extends to phantom seeing and phantom hearing. Patients with cataracts or damage to visual processing circuitry report highly detailed visual perceptions (15 percent of patients who suffer partial or complete vision loss report visual phantoms). Deaf patients often report noises (tinnitus) which range from loud, unpleasant sounds (like whistling or clanging) to music or voices. In all these cases, loss of normal sensory input increases neuromatrix activity which the brain transforms into perceptions.
Phantom limbs,
seeing, and hearing demonstrate that the brain constructs perceptions in
the absence of sensory inputs. (The brain does more than detect and
evaluate stimuli.) Second, phantoms show that the brain is prewired to
perceive the body and limbs. Melzack concludes: "the brain generates the
experience of the body. Sensory inputs merely modulate that experience;
they do not directly cause it" (p. 126).
The biopsychosocial model proposes
that the interplay of physical pathology, psychological processes, and social
and economic factors determines each patient's unique pain experience,
pain behavior, and physical disability. In
Gatchel's (2005) view, "the biopsychosocial model is now
viewed as the most heuristic approach; it appropriately conceptualizes
pain as a complex and dynamic interaction among physiologic, psychologic,
and social factors that often results in, or at least maintains, pain"
(p. 23).
Unremitting pain in patients diagnosed with arthritis, cancer, diabetes,
and nerve trauma lowers the firing threshold of their spinal cord
neurons. Now, weak somatosensory stimulation, like a light touch, causes
spinal cord neuron firing and body-wide pain (Melton, 2004).
Chronic pain may structurally reroute neuronal connections in the dorsal
horn of the spinal cord. Alterations in the expression of genes like c-fos
and c-jun in second-order neurons may change enzyme and
neuropeptide (e.g., nerve growth factor) release and modify synapses with
sensory neurons carrying pain information. Both structural reorganization of the dorsal horn and
volume transmission (extrasynaptic release) of neurotransmitters may
contribute to the spread of pain beyond the site of injury (Ranney,
2001).
Coull and colleagues (2005) propose
that damage to sensory neurons activates
microglia (glial immune cells).
These microglia release BDNF
(brain-derived neurotrophic factor), which reverses pain inhibition by
neurons that release GABA and glycine in lamina I of the spinal cord
where several pain pathways begin. BDNF alters the distribution of
anions (mainly Cl-) in sensory
neurons that transmit pain information. When GABA and glycine bind to
receptors on these affected neurons, they depolarize them and amplify
pain transmission. This results in allodynia,
a neuropathic pain syndrome where nonpainful stimuli, like light touch,
elicit severe pain.
Chronic pain can produce brain atrophy and impair judgment.
Apkariana
used magnetic imaging to compare the overall volume and gray matter
density of the prefrontal cortex of chronic low back pain patients and
normal volunteers. The low back pain patients showed significantly
greater cortical atrophy.
Next, he compared decision making on the Iowa Gambling Test by 26
patients who had suffered low back pain for at least one year and 29
normal volunteers. He excluded severely depressed or anxious individuals
from this study to minimize confounding. In the Iowa Gambling Test,
subjects are allowed to select cards from decks that differ in maximum
cash rewards and penalties. While normal subjects developed card
selection strategies that won money, low back pain patients randomly
selected their cards and made 40% fewer successful decisions. The low
back pain patients' performance was inversely correlated with their
self-reported suffering. Despite their decision-making deficits, they
showed no global cognitive impairment.
Apkariana found similar results with chronic Complex
Regional Pain
Syndrome (CRPS), also called reflex sympathetic dystrophy or
causalgia, which
may follow arm or leg injury (Melton, 2004).
Trigger
points are hyperirritable regions of
taut bands of skeletal muscle in the muscle belly or associated
fascia (connective tissue). Pressure on
trigger points is painful. Trigger points can produce
referred (remote)
pain and tenderness, motor dysfunction, and autonomic changes. Trigger
points cannot be detected using SEMG electrodes, but can be identified
using needle EMG electrodes and palpation
(examination by feeling or
pressing with the hand).
Hubbard and Gevirtz
have proposed that sympathetically-mediated muscle spindle spasm
may be the major
local mechanism in myofascial pain. An important implication of this
theory is that muscle spindles may be activated by stress and anxiety.
Muscle spindles detect muscle length,
tension, and pressure. They are activated by the sympathetic branch of
the autonomic nervous system when epinephrine binds to
α-1 adrenergic receptors. They are activated
by the sympathetic branch of the autonomic nervous system when
epinephrine binds to
α-1 adrenergic
receptors. Inserted EMG electrodes reveal muscle spasm in the affected
muscle fiber, shown by elevated inserted EMG amplitudes, while nearby
fibers in the same muscle are electrically silent. Consistent with this
model, intrafusal muscle spasm is terminated by α-1 adrenergic
antagonists like phentolamine and phenoxybenzamine, but not curare.
Gevirtz (2003) contends that intrafusal muscle spasm accounts for most of the
variance in chronic pain, whereas neurological factors that influence
afferent pain pathways account for a minority of the variance in chronic
pain.
Gevirtz's (2003)
mediational model of muscle pain
proposes that lack of assertiveness and resultant worry each trigger
sympathetic activation. Increased sympathetic efferent signals
to muscle spindles and overexertion can produce a spasm in the intrafusal
fibers of the muscle
spindle, increasing muscle spindle capsule pressure and causing myofascial pain.
Now that you have completed this module, explain why treating chronic
pain as if it acute can be debilitating. What is the clinical relevance
of descending pain-modulating pathways?
Andreassi, J. L. (2000). Psychophysiology: Human behavior and
physiological response. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,
Publishers.
Bear, M. F., Connors, B. W., & Paradiso (2007). Neuroscience: Exploring the brain (3rd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
Coull, J. A. M., et al. (2005). BDNF from microglia causes the shift in
neuronal anion gradient underlying neuropathic pain. Nature, 438,
1017-1021.
Craig, A. D. (2003). A new view of pain as a homeostatic emotion.
Trends in Neurosciences, 26(6), 303-307.
Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does
rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science,
302, 290-292.
Gatchel, R. J. (2005). Clinical essentials of pain management.
Washington, D. C.: The American Psychological Association.
Heuther, S. E., & McCance, K. L. (2004). Understanding pathophysiology (3rd ed.). St. Louis, MO: Mosby.
Jain, D., Burg, M., & Soufer, R. (1995). Prognostic implications of
mental stress induced silent ventricular dysfunction in patients with
stable angina pectoris. American Journal of Cardiology, 76, 30-35.
Kalat, J. W. (2004). Biological psychology (8th ed.). Belmont, CA:
Wadsworth.
Kingsley, R. E. (2000). Concise textbook of neuroscience (2nd
ed.). Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
Melton, L. (2004). Aching atrophy. Scientific American, 290(1),
22-24.
Melzack, R. (1992). Phantom limbs. Scientific American, 266(4),
120-126.
Melzack, R., & Wall, P. D. (1965). Pain mechanisms: A new theory.
Science, 150, 971-979.
Petrovic, P., Kalso, E., Petersson, K. M., & Ingvar, M. (2002). Placebo
and opioid analgesia--Imaging a shared neuronal network. Science,
295, 1737-1740.
Pinel, J. P. (2006). Biopsychology (6th ed.). Boston: Pearson
Education, Inc.
Ranney, D. (2001). Paper presented at the Ontario Inter-Urban Pain
Conference.
M. S.
Schwartz, & F. Andrasik (Eds.). (2003). Biofeedback: A practitioner's guide
(3rd ed.).
New
York: The Guilford Press.
Sherman, R. A. (2004). Pain assessment and intervention. Wheat Ridge, CO: AAPB.
Taylor, S. E. (2006). Health psychology (6th ed.). New
York: McGraw-Hill.
Tortora, G. J., & Derrickson, B. H. (2006). Principles of anatomy and
physiology (11th ed.). New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Wager, T. D., Rilling, J. K., Smith, E. E., Sokolik, A., Casey, K. L.,
Davidson, R. J., Kosslyn, S. M., Rose, R. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2004).
Placebo-induced changes in fMRI in the anticipation and experience of
pain. Science, 303, 1162-1167.
Wickramasekera, I. A. (1988). Clinical behavioral medicine: Some concepts
and procedures. New York: Plenum Press.
Wilson, J. (2003). Biological foundations of human behavior. Belmont, CA:
Wadsworth/Thompson Learning.